[h=3]Neuropsychology[/h]Changes in brain activity have been found in some studies of highly responsive hypnotic subjects. These changes vary depending upon the type of suggestions being given.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-168"][168][/URL][URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-169"][169][/URL] The state of light to medium hypnosis, where the body undergoes physical and mental relaxation, is associated with a pattern mostly of alpha waves
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-170"][170][/URL] However, what these results indicate is unclear. They may indicate that suggestions genuinely produce changes in perception or experience that are not simply a result of imagination. However, in normal circumstances without hypnosis, the brain regions associated with motion detection are activated both when motion is seen and when motion is imagined, without any changes in the subjects' perception or experience.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-171"][171][/URL] This may therefore indicate that highly suggestible hypnotic subjects are simply activating to a greater extent the areas of the brain used in imagination, without real perceptual changes. It is, however, premature to claim that hypnosis and meditation are mediated by similar brain systems and neural mechanisms.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-172"][172][/URL]
Another study has demonstrated that a colour hallucination suggestion given to subjects in hypnosis activated colour-processing regions of the occipital cortex.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-173"][173][/URL] A 2004 review of research examining the
EEG laboratory work in this area concludes:
Hypnosis is not a unitary state and therefore should show different patterns of EEG activity depending upon the task being experienced. In our evaluation of the literature, enhanced
theta is observed during hypnosis when there is task performance or concentrative hypnosis, but not when the highly hypnotizable individuals are passively relaxed, somewhat sleepy and/or more diffuse in their attention.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-174"][174][/URL]
Studies have shown an association of hypnosis with stronger theta-frequency activity as well as with changes to the
gamma-frequency activity.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-175"][175][/URL] Neuroimaging techniques have been used to investigate neural correlates of hypnosis.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-176"][176][/URL][URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-177"][177][/URL]
The induction phase of hypnosis may also affect the activity in brain regions that control
intention and process
conflict. Anna Gosline claims:
Gruzelier and his colleagues studied brain activity using an
fMRI while subjects completed a standard cognitive exercise, called the
Stroop task. The team screened subjects before the study and chose 12 that were highly susceptible to hypnosis and 12 with low susceptibility. They all completed the task in the fMRI under normal conditions and then again under hypnosis. Throughout the study, both groups were consistent in their task results, achieving similar scores regardless of their mental state. During their first task session, before hypnosis, there were no significant differences in brain activity between the groups. But under hypnosis, Gruzelier found that the highly susceptible subjects showed significantly more brain activity in the
anterior cingulate gyrus than the weakly susceptible subjects. This area of the brain has been shown to respond to errors and evaluate emotional outcomes. The highly susceptible group also showed much greater brain activity on the left side of the
prefrontal cortex than the weakly susceptible group. This is an area involved with higher level cognitive processing and behaviour.
[URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-178"][178][/URL][URL="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hypnosis#cite_note-179"][179][/URL]